The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves (2024)

The first thing to understand about our inability to self-tickle is that it’s just one example of a widespread phenomenon: humans respond differently to touch depending on whether the sensation was created by ourselves or something else.

If you clap your hands, then have someone else clap one of your hands with theirs, you will generally perceive the latter as more intense. This difference in how we perceive ourselves and other things in the environment isn’t limited to humans, or to touch. In 2003, a study showed that crickets perceive their own chirps as quieter than those of other crickets.

Having this ability makes sense in evolutionary terms, says Dr Konstantina Kilteni at the Karolinska Institute in Stockholm, Sweden. It’s useful to know if a sensation is worth paying attention to or not. ‘If you have a bug crawling up your arm, you want to be sure you notice that,’ she said.

Body ownership

A prerequisite to this is that our brains have a sense of body ownership, so that we know whether a touch comes from our own moving fingers, say, or some foreign object. Understanding how this works is probably a crucial part of getting to grips with tickling. Dr Kilteni says that a raft of studies began to probe this in the late 1990s, but while they established a link between the intensity of touch and where it originates, they didn’t explore the precise conditions for this. She began the Tickle Me project in 2017 to go deeper.

One of her key experiments involved looking at the way people perceived touches on their fingers using a clever set up of levers. In the first part of the experiment, people touched a lever with their left forefinger, which instantly triggered a second lever to touch their right forefinger.

Dr Kilteni then compared this with two variations. In the first, people let their left finger rest on a plate above the first lever, then the plate was removed letting the finger fall onto the lever. This triggered the second lever to touch the right finger, but crucially this was now involuntary. In a final variation, the right finger was touched by the lever without any input from the person at all. It turned out that people perceived the touches generated by these three methods as successively more intense, even though they were all made with the same force. This suggests that if the brain knows a touch is coming, it feels it as less intense. This confirms that one of the reasons we cannot tickle ourselves is because our brain has already planned it, says Dr Kilteni.

In a separate experiment that used the same lever equipment, Dr Kilteni also introduced a sneaky twist so that when the participants touched the first lever with one finger, there was a delay of a fraction of a second before the second lever touched their other finger. It turned out that this element of surprise was important; the delay made the sensation more intense. All this gives us another hint as to why self-tickling is so hard: when you tickle yourself it is hard to be caught unaware.

‘You have to get kind of rough with the rats to get them to laugh; it’s rough play they like.’

Dr Kilteni conducted a raft of experiments like this during her project, but perhaps the most telling paper she has produced came out just a few months ago and concerns an area of the brain called the somatosensory cortex, a part of the brain that receives sensory information from the body.

In one experiment she had 30 volunteers touch their index fingers together, and then separately have their fingers touched by a robot, while she scanned their brains using an fMRI machine. Some people seemed to perceive the self-touch as less intense than others, and Dr Kilteni could see that these individuals tended to have stronger connections between the somatosensory cortex and another area of the brain called the cerebellum.

Little brain

The cerebellum, or ‘little brain’, is found at the nape of the neck. It is central to the control of our bodies’ movements but it is also thought to play a crucial role overseeing cognitive processing. Think of the brain like a factory with different parts processing different information and the cerebellum is the quality control supervisor. Neuroscientists suspect that the cerebellum sends signals to dial down the perception of tickling in the somatosensory cortex when it is our own fingers, not someone else’s, at work. Dr Kilteni’s fMRI studies lend weight to that hypothesis.

Over in New Jersey, US, DrMarlies Oostland is planning to further probe this connection through her NeuroTick project. One of Dr Oostland’s project supervisors, Professor Michael Brecht, at the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience at Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany, was the scientist who along with his colleague Dr Shimpei Ishiyama, discovered that rats are ticklish in 2016. They showed that when tickled, rats emit ultrasonic ‘laughs’ and that their somatosensory cortex lights up like a Christmas tree at the same time.

Tickling the rats didn’t come entirely naturally to Oostland when she had a go on a visit to Berlin. ‘I’m used to working with mice, so I was too gentle,’ she said. ‘You have to get kind of rough with the rats to get them to laugh; it’s rough play they like.’

Dr Oostland is beginning her project at Princeton University by making fundamental studies of how the cerebellum in mice predicts the animals’ movements. She is using probes to measure the activity of individual cells in the cerebellum of a mouse to understand what’s going on in its brain as she puffs air at their whiskers (which isn’t unpleasant but should be surprising).

Armed with this understanding, the plan is for her to then move to Prof. Brecht’s lab in Germany in two year’s time to study the connection between the cerebellum and somatosensory cortex and try to confirm whether and how the signals pass between the two.

As well as helping us build a better fundamental understanding of the most sophisticated object in the universe, the human brain, Dr Oostland says work like this could help us understand autism spectrum disorder better too. People who have an injury to the cerebellum soon after birth have a 36 times higher chance of developing autism later in life. We don’t fully understand why, but Dr Oostaland says fundamental studies like this could help.

The research in this article was funded by the EU. If you liked this article, please consider sharing it on social media.

The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves (2024)

FAQs

The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves? ›

Our studies at University College London have shown that the cerebellum can predict sensations when your own movement causes them but not when someone else does. When you try to tickle yourself, the cerebellum predicts the sensation and this prediction is used to cancel the response of other brain areas to the tickle.

Why are we not ticklish when we tickle ourselves? ›

When somebody else tickles you, your muscles haven't got a plan from your brain, so the feeling is surprising – and ticklish! But you can't tickle yourself, because your brain is always one step ahead, telling your muscles and senses what to expect and stopping you from giving yourself a surprise.

Why is it possible to tickle someone else but not to tickle yourself? ›

The reason you can't tickle yourself is that when you move a part of your own body, a part of your brain monitors the movement and anticipates the sensations that it will cause.

What does tickling do to the brain? ›

During a tickle, the skin's nerve endings shoot electrical signals to the somatosensory cortex, a part of the brain that processes touch. Meanwhile, the anterior cingulate cortex analyzes these signals as either harmful or playful. But in the back part of the brain, the cerebellum gives you away.

What is the psychology behind tickling? ›

It appears that the tickle sensation involves signals from nerve fibres associated with both pain and touch. Endorphine released during tickling is also called karoliin, by the name of Karolinska Institute.

What is the science behind not being ticklish? ›

Dr. Vyas says that as you age, your reduction in ticklishness may be due to changes in nerve function and sensitivity. It could also be that children often see tickling as a fun, playful interaction, while adults find it to be more invasive and less enjoyable.

How rare is it to be able to tickle yourself? ›

Most people cannot tickle themselves. However, some individuals with schizophrenia may be able to do so, possibly because they are less aware of the consequences of their movements. Schizophrenia is a condition that can affect a person's thinking, speech, movement, and perception of the world.

Is it physically impossible to tickle yourself? ›

Pressure on certain sensitive parts of the body can lead to a tickling sensation. Yet even the most ticklish person will have a hard time trying to tickle themselves. That's because our brains anticipate our touch, effectively canceling out our own tickles.

Can psychopaths be tickled? ›

The average psychopath or sociopath is no less ticklish then a neurotypical. However, we tend to be better at ignoring the unpleasant feeling and pretending that it's not there. Personally, tickling doesn't bother me. I understand that I'm being tickled, but that's it.

What percentage of humans are not ticklish? ›

While many people assume that other people enjoy tickling, a recent survey of 84 college students indicated that only 32% of respondents enjoy being tickled, with 32% giving neutral responses and 36% stating that they do not enjoy being tickled.

Can too much tickling be harmful? ›

“Excessive tickling can also lead to anxiety in children. Not only this, but tickling can even cause death from asphyxia, brain aneurysms or other stress-related injuries when done constantly. It is better to avoid tickling the baby to make sure that there are no problems due to it,” said Dr Kathwate.

Why can't you tickle babies' feet? ›

That's because, according to new evidence, infants in the first four months of life apparently feel that touch and wiggle their feet without connecting the sensation to you. When you tickle the toes of newborn babies, the experience for them isn't quite as you would imagine it to be.

What happens if you are tickled for 24 hours? ›

It could cause you both mental and physical harm. At the 24 hour Mark, you'd have lost control of your body. and you wouldn't be able to regulate your breathing, and you'd be very stressed. You could have asphyxia, a brain aneurysm or a heart attack.

Is tickling good for Mental Health? ›

Some of the benefits of tickling include: Stress management: Tickling generates a sense of well-being. It can help reduce stress and anxiety.

What was tickling originally used for? ›

In ancient China, tickle torture was used as a way to punish the noble class. It was preferred over other types of discipline because it left no marks and the victim would recover fairly quickly. It was also used as a torture tool in ancient Japan, where it was called merciless tickling.

What does tickling a girl mean? ›

Key points. Tickling often begins as childhood play but can take on different meanings as we grow into adulthood. For example, tickling can be harmless, fun and playful, sexual, unwanted, or even a reminder of previous trauma.

Where is the only place you can tickle yourself? ›

Tickle the roof of your mouth with your tongue.

Lightly rotate your tongue in a circle on the roof of your mouth to create a tickling sensation. No one is entirely sure why this method works, since the areas of our brain that process sensation are less active when self-tickling.

Why you shouldn't tickle people? ›

The main thing that makes tickling problematic is that children may not be able to say when they want it to stop. Laughter is an automatic response to being touched by a tickler—it's not a response that the child can opt out of. This puts the tickler in charge of how much or how long the child laughs.

Why don't humans like being tickled? ›

People may hate being tickled due to the loss of control over their bodies, experts say. Tickling can overwhelm the nervous system, causing actual, if temporary, paralysis, Alan Fridlund, Ph. D., associate professor in the department of psychological and brain sciences at the University of California, told Vice.

Top Articles
NZXT Goes Full Rent-A-Center by Launching Subscription PC Service
A North Korean Hacker Tricked a US Security Vendor Into Hiring Him—and Immediately Tried to Hack Them
Devin Mansen Obituary
Jeremy Corbell Twitter
Vaya Timeclock
Nm Remote Access
Waive Upgrade Fee
Xm Tennis Channel
Missing 2023 Showtimes Near Landmark Cinemas Peoria
Günstige Angebote online shoppen - QVC.de
OSRS Dryness Calculator - GEGCalculators
Oro probablemente a duna Playa e nomber Oranjestad un 200 aña pasa, pero Playa su historia ta bay hopi mas aña atras
Lesson 8 Skills Practice Solve Two-Step Inequalities Answer Key
Kvta Ventura News
Pickswise Review 2024: Is Pickswise a Trusted Tipster?
Panic! At The Disco - Spotify Top Songs
Busted Campbell County
Tripadvisor Napa Restaurants
Drift Hunters - Play Unblocked Game Online
Craigslist Lake Charles
Student Portal Stvt
Amerisourcebergen Thoughtspot 2023
Jesus Revolution Showtimes Near Regal Stonecrest
Jackie Knust Wendel
Bleacher Report Philadelphia Flyers
Is Light Raid Hard
Maine Racer Swap And Sell
Hobby Lobby Hours Parkersburg Wv
Shiny Flower Belinda
Armor Crushing Weapon Crossword Clue
Missing 2023 Showtimes Near Mjr Southgate
Little Caesars Saul Kleinfeld
Tra.mypatients Folio
Royals op zondag - "Een advertentie voor Center Parcs" of wat moeten we denken van de laatste video van prinses Kate?
4083519708
Afspraak inzien
Laurin Funeral Home | Buried In Work
Dmitri Wartranslated
Rochester Ny Missed Connections
Lbl A-Z
Author's Purpose And Viewpoint In The Dark Game Part 3
Www Craigslist Com Atlanta Ga
Spreading Unverified Info Crossword Clue
Air Sculpt Houston
The Many Faces of the Craigslist Killer
3367164101
Jigidi Jigsaw Puzzles Free
Bumgarner Funeral Home Troy Nc Obituaries
Psalm 46 New International Version
Ok-Selection9999
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Nathanael Baumbach

Last Updated:

Views: 6575

Rating: 4.4 / 5 (75 voted)

Reviews: 82% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Nathanael Baumbach

Birthday: 1998-12-02

Address: Apt. 829 751 Glover View, West Orlando, IN 22436

Phone: +901025288581

Job: Internal IT Coordinator

Hobby: Gunsmithing, Motor sports, Flying, Skiing, Hooping, Lego building, Ice skating

Introduction: My name is Nathanael Baumbach, I am a fantastic, nice, victorious, brave, healthy, cute, glorious person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.